Open access peer-reviewed chapter - ONLINE FIRST

The Influence of Affect in Help-Seeking Behaviors and Performance in a Math Intelligent Tutoring System

Written By

Ana Paula S. Loures-Elias and Matthew L. Bernacki

Submitted: 13 November 2023 Reviewed: 28 December 2023 Published: 09 February 2024

DOI: 10.5772/intechopen.1004185

Artificial Intelligence for Quality Education IntechOpen
Artificial Intelligence for Quality Education Edited by Seifedine Kadry

From the Edited Volume

Artificial Intelligence and Education - Shaping the Future of Learning [Working Title]

Dr. Seifedine Kadry

Chapter metrics overview

67 Chapter Downloads

View Full Metrics

Abstract

This study investigates the association between help-seeking behaviors (hints, hints per step, hints with steps requests, and hint to error), affect (boredom, confusion, frustration, happiness, and engagement), and performance in seventh and eighth-grade students using the Cognitive Tutor Bridge to Algebra as a self-regulated learning environment. Analyses focused on correlations between students’ help-seeking behaviors and their affect in units 4 and 14. Affect was also used to predict help-seeking behaviors in the next units (e.g., 5 and 15). Moreover, we examined how associations between help-seeking behaviors and performance differed as a function of affect. The results showed that a pattern emerged in which students showed more executive help-seeking behaviors rather than instrumental ones. Students feeling bored, confused, and frustrated tended to use more hints, and they were less likely to switch to external help-seeking sources. Also, those feeling happy or engaged were less likely to use hints.

Keywords

  • help-seeking
  • affect
  • academic emotions
  • math intelligent tutoring system
  • performance

1. Introduction

Help-seeking is a self-regulatory strategy in self-regulated learning (SRL) in which learners use help-seeking to sustain cognition, behavior, affect, and motivational factors in order to achieve goals in a domain-specific activity [1, 2]. According to Aleven [1], help-seeking is defined “[…] as episodes in which a learner, in the context of a specific learning activity […] takes the initiative to seek assistance from a source within or outside of the learning environment, as opposed to persisting at trying to make progress independently” (p. 312). Help-seeking can be an instrumental (adaptive) strategy, in which learners seek to learn with understanding, or executive (maladaptive), when learners seek prompted answers without developing understanding [1, 3].

Previous research shows that help-seeking has been studied within the math domain [4, 5, 6, 7, 8, 9, 10, 11]. Du and colleagues [12] showed that help-seeking was positively associated with math homework interest at the individual and class levels in eighth graders. The role of peers is a significant predictor of help-seeking in middle and high school students [11, 13, 14, 15, 16]. Perfectionism and feedback were also positively related to help-seeking behaviors [4, 10, 17, 18]. Adaptative help-seeking behaviors significantly predicted mastery-approach goals in middle and high school students [13, 19]. In addition, perceived parent achievement goals predicted students’ help-seeking and help-avoidance behaviors in middle school students’ achievement goals [6]. Regarding gender, female students are more likely to demonstrate adaptative help-seeking behaviors, whereas boys are more likely to exhibit executive help-seeking behaviors [8, 15, 18, 20].

Help is a typical feature in intelligent tutoring systems (ITSs), and it is usually provided at students’ request through the form of hints, internal help-seeking sources, or switches, and external help-seeking sources such as glossaries or hyperlinks. Previous research showed that high school students in a math help forum adopted executive help-seeking behaviors [9]. Best and colleagues [21] demonstrated that social support was increased and stigma was reduced by adolescent males seeking help online. Roll and colleagues [7] showed that high school students asking for help in online Geometry Cognitive Tutor had more productive learning, whereas those who overused it had poor learning. Contrary to previous research, avoiding help was associated with better learning. Otieno and colleagues [5] showed that hints and glossaries in the Cognitive Tutor Geometry differed from the answers in the questionnaire they gave to high school students, yet with a high prediction of learning. Roll and colleagues [4] showed that high school students were able to transfer their learning from a Geometry Cognitive Tutor to a new domain-level content.

The decision to seek help is influenced by metacognitive, affective, and social competencies. Affect can be understood “[…] as a generic term that includes emotions, mood, feelings, attitudes, etc.” (p. 65) [22]. Students are likely to experience boredom, confusion, frustration, happiness, and engagement in intelligent learning environments (ILE) because they are affectively charged experiences [23]. Previous research shows that affect has been studied within the math domain. Positive academic emotions such as engagement, enjoyment, joy, contentment, and hope have been associated with adaptative behavior and lower math difficulties. However, negative academic emotions such as anxiety, confusion, frustration, shame, and hopelessness have been associated with executive behaviors and higher math difficulties [24, 25, 26, 27, 28, 29, 30]. Academic emotions and learning motivation were also studied in which students were more motivated when they exhibited positive academic emotions such as hope, pride, and enjoyment [31, 32, 33]. In addition, students exhibiting higher enjoyment and lower boredom had higher subsequent achievement, but pride was negatively correlated with achievement [29, 34].

Affect influences learning outcomes in face-to-face and mediated learning environments. Previous research has studied the effect of ITS on affect [33, 35, 36, 37, 38, 39, 40, 41, 42, 43]. In addition, an ITS provided transition from negative to positive affective states in high school students, and it improved their frustration and confidence by using online learning companions [24]. San Pedro and colleagues [25] showed that high school students who felt more engaged made more careless mistakes and those who felt confused or bored made fewer careless mistakes. Pre-school children demonstrated engagement and valence behaviors to nonverbal behaviors from the social robot tutor [44]. Padrón-Rivera and colleagues [28] showed in a math action units study that high school students experiencing confusion had better learning than those experiencing frustration.

However, to the best of our knowledge, no research was found about the influence of affect in students’ help-seeking behaviors in math ITSs until the present date. Thus, more research is needed on how students’ affect and motivation may influence learners’ decisions to seek or not to seek help in ITSs [45]. According to D’Mello [23], there is still the need to understand “[…] how affective states arise, morph, decay, and impact learning outcomes […]” (p. 2).

The purpose of this study was to examine affect and its implications for help-seeking as an SRL process. According to D’Mello [23], “Affective impacts of technologically infused learning environments are not very well understood” (p. 2). In addition, Karabenick and Gonida [3] acknowledged the need for additional research to better understand help-seeking as an SRL process. We aimed to investigate how students’ academic emotions correlated with help-seeking within one task and if affect was a significant predictor of help-seeking on a subsequent task. Thereafter, we examined the effects of help-seeking on performance and the ways affect moderates this relationship. We attempted to answer the following research questions: (1) does students’ affect after learning correlate with their help-seeking behavior within a unit of math problem-solving? (2) Is students’ affect reported in one unit significant predictor of help-seeking in the next? (3) How do students’ help-seeking behaviors and affect relate to their performance in a consecutive unit of math problem-solving tasks? We also hypothesized that, first, students presenting academic emotions such as happiness and engagement are less likely to switch to external help-seeking sources and make errors, and they are more likely to use hints as an internal help-seeking source and respond correctly to the questions. Secondly, students presenting academic emotions such as boredom, confusion, and frustration are less likely to use hints and respond correctly to the questions, and they are more likely to switch to external help-seeking sources and make errors.

Advertisement

2. Method

2.1 Participants

One hundred and ten students from math courses at a suburban, public middle school participated in the study. Students were 50% male and 50% female, and 96% were Caucasian. 71.8% reported low socioeconomic status (SES), and they were eligible for a free or reduced lunch from the school. Students were primarily enrolled in the seventh grade, but there were students from the eighth grade. Participation occurred in two math periods per week (45–50 minutes each) across the whole academic year as part of regular classroom activities through online questionnaires about math exercises, their academic emotions, and motivation using the Cognitive Tutor Bridge to Algebra [46].

2.2 Materials

The Cognitive Tutor Bridge to Algebra [47] collected fine-grained traced data about students’ help-seeking behaviors in pre-algebra exercises in 54 units being units 4, 5, 14, and 15 used for this study, and it presented online questionnaires with a Likert scale ranging from 1 to 5 for students to report their academic emotions starting with boredom, confusion, frustration, happiness, and engagement, respectively [2, 48]. The unit topics comprised “Lowest Common Multiple” in unit 4, “Greatest Common Factor” in unit 5, “Converting Fractions to Decimals” in unit 14, and “Decimal Operations” in unit 15. Units 4, 5, 14, and 15 were chosen because they represented the most challenging ones for students in which the difficulty was operationalized by time per unit and time per problem.

2.3 Procedures

This study focused on unit and problem level assessments employing a microgenetic and longitudinal approach. In the microgenetic approach, transactions were examined at unit and problem levels to understand the relationship between help-seeking, affect, and performance [2]. Researching at the transaction level allows to correlate help-seeking tools to affect and how the latter may predict the former in consecutive units. With longitudinal and microgenetic data, it is possible to determine if there is a correlation between help-seeking and academic emotions and how this correlation changes when it is investigated at unit and problem levels [2, 48].

In order to investigate the unit and problem level assessments, the Cognitive Tutor Bridge to Algebra uses the model of desired help-seeking behavior developed by Aleven and colleagues [49]. Briefly, students should start by thinking about the problem. If the problem is familiar and they know what to do, students make an attempt. If the problem is familiar but they do not know what to do, students switch to use the glossary to learn more about the problem. If they know what to do after consulting the glossary, students make an attempt. If not, they may either use the glossary again or ask for a hint. If the problem is not familiar, students ask for a hint, spend time reading it, and decide if the hint is helpful. If yes, they make an attempt. If not, students ask for another hint. If students make a correct attempt, the problem is complete. If not, students may require another hint and/or make a new attempt. In this study, hints reflect the total number of hints requested in raw terms. Hints per step normalize these requests to the amount of problem steps students had to complete. Steps with hints normalize the hint requests to account for steps in which students ask for one or more hints. Hint to error ratio reflects the proportion of times students opted to use a hint, rather than to make an attempt at a step that ultimately yielded an incorrect answer. Switch reflects the total number of glossary requests in raw terms [45, 49].

Advertisement

3. Results

Analyses focused on correlations between students’ help-seeking behaviors and their affect in units 4 and 14. Affect was also used to predict help-seeking behaviors in the next units (e.g., 5 and 15). Moreover, we examined how associations between help-seeking behaviors and performance differed as a function of affect. The alpha level was set at .05 for simple correlations and multiple regressions.

Missing data were observed on 28 of the 34 variables examined. In unit 4, 20.9% of the data were missing on the boredom variable, 20% of data were missing on confusion, 19.1% of data were missing on frustration, happiness, engagement, hints, hints per step, steps with hint requests, and hint to error. In unit 5, 19.1% of data were missing on performance, hints, hints per step, steps with hint requests, and hint to error. In unit 14, 18.2% of data were missing on boredom, confusion, and frustration, 20% of data were missing on happiness and engagement, 12.7% of data were missing on hints, hints per step, steps with hint requests, and hint to error. In unit 15, 11.8% of data were missing on performance, hints, hints per step, steps with hint requests, and hint to error.

Rubin [50] proposed a taxonomy of missing data mechanisms to explain why the choice of missing data method impacts model parameters. The missing data were found to be consistent with a missing completely at random (MCAR) mechanism as determined by Little’s [51] omnibus MCAR test, d2(199) = 213.907, p = .223. Therefore, listwise deletion (N = 97) was used to handle missingness for the present research problems because it is in accordance with the MCAR mechanism and it does not bias the parameter estimates generated by the correlations and multiple regression analyses. Table 1 shows the descriptive statistics for affect, help-seeking behaviors, and performance in units 4, 5, 14, and 15. Happiness and engagement had the highest coefficients in units 4 and 14. Hints also had the highest coefficients with the exception of unit 5 in which switch exceeded hints. This may be due to the highest level of difficulty of the unit. Moreover, it demonstrated that students favored the usage of hints as an adaptative help-seeking behavior.

Unit 4Unit 5Unit 14Unit 15
MSDMSDMSDMSD
Affect
Boredom4.701.844.941.76
Confusion4.081.974.801.97
Frustration3.812.194.542.01
Happiness5.831.195.841.25
Engagement5.431.485.451.47
help-seeking behaviors
Switch0.541.9419.2514.353.725.690.881.96
Hints6.3411.456.3411.4539.1277.9411.0313.02
Hints per step0.220.040.220.040.130.140.030.03
Steps with hints requested274.82115.77274.82115.77227.8110.8332.62113.11
Hint to error0.180.240.180.240.770.770.260.23
Performance0.940.030.930.03

Table 1.

Descriptive statistics for help-seeking behaviors and affect in units.

Note. Listwise N = 110.

Table 2 answers the first research question if students’ affect after learning correlates with their help-seeking behavior within a unit of math problem-solving. It shows correlations between affect components and help-seeking behaviors in units 4 and 14. In contrast to hypotheses one and two and the assumptions by Karabenick [52], a positive moderate correlation of .213 between hints and boredom was found in unit 4. Students feeling boredom were more likely to use hints in unit 4 as an instrumental help-seeking behavior. However, this correlation was nonsignificant in unit 14. Negative moderate correlations of −.218 and − .209 between happiness and hints per step and between happiness and steps with hints requests, respectively, were found in unit 14. They are also in contrast with hypotheses one and two. Students feeling happy were less likely to use hints per step and steps with hints requested in unit 14. It might indicate an executive type of help-seeking behavior. No other correlations were significant in units 4 and 14.

VariableSwitchHintsHints per stepSteps with hints requestedHint to error
Unit 4
Boredom0.097.213*0.1890.1430.094
Confusion0.0340.1860.1640.1020.017
Frustration0.1070.0650.1090.021−0.051
Happiness−0.182−0.064−0.0890.012−0.038
Engagement−0.110−0.022−0.008−0.039−0.029
Unit 14
Boredom−0.062−0.025−0.001−0.031−0.046
Confusion−0.044−0.0430.0000.027−0.042
Frustration−0.0010.0330.0720.090.053
Happiness−0.146−0.197−.218*−.209*−0.186
Engagement−0.039−0.108−0.137−0.09−0.128

Table 2.

Correlations between affect components and help-seeking behaviors in units.

p = 0.05.


Note. Listwise N = 87.

Multiple linear regression analyses with ordinary least squares estimations were conducted to answer the second research question. It was investigated if students’ affect (boredom, confusion, frustration, happiness, and engagement) reported in units 4 and 14 were significant predictors of help-seeking behaviors (hints, hints per step, steps with hints requested, hint to error ratio, and switch) in the next units 5 and 15, respectively (Table 3).

VariableFDfpR2bSEβp
Unit 5 from unit 4
Switch1.3015,800.2720.075
From boredom1.1940.9520.1810.214
From confusion1.4250.9700.2280.146
From frustration−1.4620.878−0.2600.100
From happiness0.3181.2380.0310.798
From engagement−1.0861.012−0.1320.287
Hints2.7685,800.0230.148
From boredom3.3441.1390.4060.004*
From confusion0.5981.1610.0770.608
From frustration−2.134,1.050−0.3040.045*
From happiness1.6221.480.1270.276
From engagement−2.2531.211−0.220.067
Hints per step5.1011.850.0260.057
From boredom0.0020.0010.2380.026*
From confusion−0.0010.001−0.0770.618
From frustration0.0000.001−0.0530.730
From happiness0.0020.0020.0960.425
From engagement−0.0020.002−0.1740.157
Steps with hints requested0.9555,800.4500.056
From boredom11.98424.3360.0720.624
From confusion36.74524.8030.2320.142
From frustration−40.52122.436−0.2850.075
From happiness−11.45531.631−0.0440.718
From engagement−14.93725.879−0.0720.565
Hints to error1.2745,800.2830.074
From boredom0.0390.0190.3000.041
From confusion−0.0210.019−0.1710.273
From frustration0.0060.0170.0520.738
From happiness0.0100.0240.0520.670
From engagement−0.0240.020−0.1500.227
Unit 15 from unit 14
Switch0.0875,810.9940.005
From boredom−0.0160.205−0.0130.937
From confusion−0.1250.252−0.1160.621
From frustration0.1030.2390.0950.667
From happiness0.0720.2390.0420.764
From engagement−0.0320.206−0.0220.877
Hints2.6395,810.0290.140
From boredom−1.7351.133−0.2390.13
From confusion0.1351.3920.0210.23
From frustration2.6791.3240.4140.046*
From happiness−1.3261.321−0.130.319
From engagement−0.2821.139−0.0330.805
Hints per step3.0185,810.0150.157
From boredom−0.0060.003−0.320.041*
From confusion0.0010.0030.0780.720
From frustration0.0070.0030.4300.037*
From happiness−0.0020.003−0.0980.447
From engagement−0.0010.003−0.0390.764
Steps with hints requested0.8555,810.5150.05
From boredom−5.01710.788−0.0760.643
From confusion3.99613.2550.0690.764
From frustration9.50312.6120.1620.453
From happiness−11.06612.581−0.120.382
From engagement1.45910.8440.0190.893
Hints to error4.4835,810.0010.217
From boredom−0.0750.019−0.576<.001*
From confusion0.0130.0240.110.598
From frustration0.0590.0230.5060.011*
From happiness−0.0010.023−0.0050.965
From engagement−0.0040.02−0.0260.836

Table 3.

Multiple linear regression predicting help-seeking behaviors from affect.

p < or = .05.


The first significant regression equation was found when predicting hints in unit 5 from affect components in unit 4 (F(5, 80) = 2.768, p = .023, r2 = .148). Boredom reported in unit 4 was a statistically significant predictor of the total number of hints requested in unit 5 (b = 3.344, SE = 1.139, p = .004, 95% CI = 1.077, 5.610; β = .406), such that, for each unit increase in boredom, total hint requests were predicted to increase by 3.344. Frustration reported in unit 4 was also found to be a statistically significant predictor of hints in unit 5 (b = −2.134, SE = 1.050, p = .045, 95% CI = −4.224, −.044; β = −.304). For each unit increase in frustration, students were predicted to request 2.134 fewer hints. Predicting hints per step in unit 5 from boredom in unit 4 yielded another significant regression equation (F(1,85) = 5.101, p = .026, r2 = .057). Boredom reported in unit 4 was a statistically significant predictor of hints per step in unit 5 (b = .002, SE = .001, p = .026, 95% CI = .000, .005; β = .238), such that, for each unit increase in boredom, hints per step were predicted to increase by .002 points. Models regressing steps with hints, hint to error ratio, and switch on reported affect components were not statistically significant, F(5,80) = 1.301, p = .272, r2 = .075. Collectively, a pattern emerged where boredom increased tendency toward help-seeking, while frustration led to help avoidance. It demonstrates that frustration was the only significant predictor that corroborates with hypothesis two but not with hypothesis one, in which students feeling frustrated predicted to be less likely to use hints and respond correctly to questions in the subsequent unit. We next examined the stability of relations in units 14 and 15.

When predicting hints in unit 15 from frustration in unit 14, a significant regression equation was found (F(5,81) = 2.639, p = .029, r2 = .140). Frustration reported in unit 14 was a statistically significant predictor of hints in unit 15 (b = 2.679, SE = 1.324, p = .046, 95% CI = .044, 5.314; β = .414), such that, for each unit increase in frustration, hints were predicted to increase by 2.679 points. Another significant regression equation was found (F(5,81) = 3.018, p = .015, r2 = .157) when predicting hints per step in unit 15 from affect components in unit 14. Boredom reported in unit 14 was a statistically significant predictor of hints per step in unit 15 (b = −.006, SE = .003, p = .041, 95% CI = −.011, .000; β = −.320). For each unit increase in boredom, hints per step were predicted to decrease by .006 points. Frustration reported in unit 14 was also found to be a statistically significant predictor of hints per step in unit 15 (b = .007, SE = .003, p = .037, 95% CI = .000, .013; β = .430), such that, for each unit increase in frustration, hints per step were predicted to increase by .007 points. The final significant regression equation was found (F(5,81) = 4.483, p = .001, r2 = .217) when predicting hint to error in unit 15 from affect components in unit 14. Boredom reported in unit 14 was a statistically significant predictor of hint to error in unit 15 (b = −.075, SE = .019, p < .001, 95% CI = −.114, −.037; β = −.576), such that, for each unit increase in boredom, hint to error were predicted to decrease by .075 points. Frustration reported in unit 14 was also found to be a statistically significant predictor of hint to error in unit 15 (b = .059, SE = .023, p = .011, 95% CI = .014, .104; β = .506), such that, for each unit increase in frustration, students’ use of hints rather than to make an attempt at a step that resulted in an error was predicted to increase by .059 points. The pattern that emerged was flipped in units 14 and 15 in which frustration increased the tendency toward help-seeking, while boredom led to help avoidance. Contradicting hypotheses one and two, students feeling frustrated were predicted to be more likely to use hints and respond correctly to questions in the subsequent unit. On the contrary and in accordance with hypothesis two, students feeling bored were slightly less likely to use hints and respond correctly to questions in the subsequent unit. Models regressing steps with hints and switch on reported affect components were not statistically significant (F(5,81) = .855, p = .515, r2 = .05). All models appear in Table 3.

We next examined the third research question on whether the association between help-seeking behaviors and academic performance differs as a function of the affect. Prior to conducting substantive analyses, help-seeking behaviors were centered, and the help-seeking behaviors-by-affect components product term was generated via multiplication of centered help-seeking behaviors with the affect components. The continuous predictors, as well as their product terms, were entered into simultaneous regression models. Significant and nonsignificant regression equations were found, but only significant ones are presented in Table 4. There were no significant associations between switch as a help-seeking behavior and academic performance differing as a function of affect. It suggests that the interaction between the switch and affect was not a significant predictor of performance in subsequent units.

VariablebSEβtpFdfpR2
Unit 4 predicting performance in unit 5
Constant.946.006170.610<.0013.862(3,84).012121
Hints−.001.000−.560−2.783.007
Frustration−.002.001−.181−1.766.081
Hints × frustration.000.000.4202.086.040
Constant.945.006167.640<.0012.969(3,84).036096
Hints per step−.403.172−.553−2.349.021
Frustration−.002.001−.178−1.709.091
Hints per step
X frustration
.064.031.4842.056.043
Unit 14 predicting performance in unit 15
Constant.947.009109.715<.0015.652(3,86).001165
Hints per step−.225.073−1.076−3.080.003
Confusion−.003.002−.156−1.549.125
Hints per step
X boredom
.036.014.8692.485.015
Constant.945.008124.354<.0015.961(3,86).001172
Hints per step−.186.055−.887−3.380.001
Confusion−.002.001−.150−1.489.140
Hints per step
X confusion
.030.011.6932.633.010
Constant.946.007136.648<.0017.732(3,86)<.001212
Hints per step−.223.059−1.073−3.772<.001
Frustration−.003.001−.181−1.838.070
Hints per step
X frustration
.039.012.9063.189.002
Constant.950.009110.651<.0015.242(3,86).002155
Hints to error−.041.016−1.113−2.569.012
Boredom−.003.002−.189−1.882.030
Hints to error
X boredom
.007.004.8752.016.047
Constant.946.007135.838<.0017.213(3,86)<.001201
Hints to error−.046.014−1.270−3.385.001
Frustration−.003.001−.192−1.933.056
Hints to error
X frustration
.009.0031.0742.866.005

Table 4.

Interaction effect between help-seeking behaviors and affect in subsequent performance unit.

Significant interaction effects were found in multiple units. In unit 4, a hint × frustration interaction effect indicates that every unit increase in frustration led to a slight but statistically significant additional effect where hint use conducted while frustrated predicted an even poorer subsequent performance (e.g., as measured by the percent of first attempts at problem steps) over and above the negative effect of hint use as a help-seeking method. A hints per step × frustration interaction effect indicates that every unit increase in frustration led to a slight but statistically significant additional effect, where hints per step predicted an increase of .064 units on subsequent performance after help-seeking.

In unit 14, a hints per step × boredom interaction effect indicates that every unit increase in boredom led to a slight but statistically significant additional effect, where hints per step predicted an increase of .036 units on subsequent performance. A hints per step × confusion interaction effect indicates that every unit increase in confusion led to a slight but statistically significant additional effect, where hints per step predicted an increase of .030 units on subsequent performance. A hints per step × frustration interaction effect indicates that every unit increase in frustration led to a slight but statistically significant additional effect, where hints per step predicted an increase of .039 units on subsequent performance. A hint to error × boredom interaction effect indicates that every unit increase in boredom led to a slight but statistically significant additional effect, where greater use of hints compared to making errors predicted an increase of .007 units on subsequent performance. A hint to error × frustration interaction effect indicates that every unit increase in frustration led to a slight but statistically significant additional effect, where hint to error predicted an increase of .009 units on subsequent performance. These patterns contradicted hypotheses one and two in which students feeling bored, confused, and frustrated combined with the usage of hints predicted additional performance in subsequent units.

Subsequently, we examined how many students used switch as a help-seeking behavior in units 4, 5, 14, and 15. Table 5 shows that students used more switch in unit 4. However, there were more students who overall preferred not to use switch across the four units analyzed in this study. It corroborates with descriptive findings from Table 1 in which hints had the highest coefficients yielding students’ preference.

VariableUnit 4Unit 5Unit 14Unit 15Total
Switch users17885228185
Non-switch users93225882255

Table 5.

Use of switch as a help-seeking behavior in units 4, 5, 14, and 15.

Eventually, a series of independent t-tests were conducted in order to compare affect components including boredom, confusion, frustration, happiness, and engagement between students who used switch and those who did not use it as a help-seeking behavior. There were no systematic differences between cases with switch users and nonswitch users, according to Table 6. The group of students using switch did not have a significantly higher number of affect scores than the group of nonswitch users. These results suggest that switch does not have an effect on affect components such as boredom, confusion, frustration, happiness, and engagement. It contradicts both hypotheses stated in this article. Students presenting academic emotions such as happiness and engagement are less likely to switch to external help-seeking sources, whereas students presenting academic emotions such as boredom, confusion, and frustration are more likely to switch to external help-seeking sources.

VariablesNMSDtp
Unit 4
Boredomt(85)= −1.192.236
Switch users175.182.128
Non-switch users704.591.757
Confusiont(87) = −.637.526
Switch users174.352.290
Non-switch users724.011.895
Frustrationt(86) = −.773.442
Switch users174.182.128
Non-switch users713.722.212
Happinesst(87) = .256.799
Switch users175.761.147
Non-switch users725.851.206
Engagementt(87) = −.134.894
Switch users175.471.663
Non-switch users725.421.451
Unit 14
Boredomt(88) = .046.963
Switch users474.941.686
Non-switch users434.951.851
Confusiont(88) = .384.702
Switch users474.721.942
Non-switch users434.882.014
Frustrationt(88) = .537.593
Switch users484.441.945
Non-switch users424.672.103
Happinesst(86) = .600.550
Switch users475.771.255
Non-switch users415.931.253
Engagementt(86) = .131.896
Switch users465.431.241
Non-switch users425.481.700

Table 6.

T-tests comparing switch as a help-seeking behavior with affect in units 4 and 14.

p < or = .05.

This statistically significant result means that no effect was observed.


Advertisement

4. Discussion

This study was a response to Karabenick and Gonida’s [3] call for additional research on help-seeking as a self-regulatory process, and it also examined the ways affect influenced their help-seeking intentions, behaviors, and performances within an ITS context. Students’ affect—boredom, confusion, frustration, happiness, and engagement—related to students’ concurrent problem-solving and help-seeking behaviors, predicted future help-seeking and moderated performance.

Students were more likely to use hints in unit 4 when they felt bored. This may indicate a trace of disengagement from the task and executive help-seeking behavior [52]. It contradicted hypothesis two that students feeling bored are less likely to use hints. On the other hand, students who reported they felt more positive were less likely to select multiple hints per step and rely on hints across more steps in unit 14. It does not corroborate with hypothesis one in which students feeling happy were thought to be more likely to use hints and respond correctly to the questions. According to Karabenick’s [52] description, this may instead be construed to reflect engagement in the task and trace executive help-seeking behaviors.

When predicting future help-seeking behaviors from students’ affect reported in previous units, results showed that students feeling bored in unit 4 were more likely to use hints and hints per step in unit 5 contradicting hypothesis two. On the other hand, students feeling frustrated were less likely to use hints in the subsequent unit which corroborates with hypothesis two. In unit 14, this relationship flipped and those who felt frustrated were more likely to use hints (overall, per step, and compared to making errors) in unit 15. Those who felt bored were less likely to use hints in the subsequent unit also corroborating with hypothesis two. This may indicate executive help-seeking behaviors because students were trying to keep engaged in the task by seeking help through hints while feeling bored or frustrated which contradicted the prior hypothesis. There is a need for future research to investigate the reasons why this relationship flipped from one unit to the other. Researchers should investigate more units and more students across time which was one of the limitations of this study. We hypothesized that it may be due to the level of difficulty of the unit in which Decimal Operations in unit 14 may have led to a greater frustration than Greatest Common Factor in unit 4.

Aleven and colleagues [45] acknowledged the need to investigate whether help-seeking as an SRL component could lead to improved students’ performance in later units on ITSs. Thus, we examined whether the association between help-seeking behaviors and academic performance differs as a function of affect. In unit 4, the usage of hints when students felt greater frustration predicted an even poorer subsequent performance in unit 5 than hint usage alone. This may indicate an instrumental help-seeking behavior in unit 4 which slightly undermined students’ performance in unit 5 due to the effect of frustration. On the contrary, the interaction between hints per step and frustration led to a slight increase in unit 5 performance. This may indicate an executive help-seeking behavior in unit 4 which slightly increased students’ performance in unit 5 due to the effect of frustration. In unit 14, the association between hints per step and affect (boredom, confusion, and frustration) and between hint to error and affect (boredom and confusion) led to a slight increase in students’ performance in unit 15. These results contradicted hypotheses one and two in which students feeling happy or engaged should be more likely to use hints, but no significant interactions were found. Additionally, students feeling bored, confused, and frustrated were more likely to use hints, and it led to subsequent performance. This may also indicate an executive help-seeking behavior in which students are trying to keep engaged in the task perhaps without developing much understanding in the present unit so they can progress to the next one.

We also examined how many students used switch as a help-seeking behavior in units 4, 5, 14, and 15, and we investigated if switch had an effect on affect components such as boredom, confusion, frustration, happiness, and engagement. The results showed that students used more switch only in unit 4, Greatest Common Factor. It also demonstrated that switch did not yield a significant effect on affect components. We hypothesized that it may be because students may consult other sources even if they know the answer to reach full certainty. It could also be because they were not aware of how helpful this resource could be in an ITS. There is also the need to investigate switches, as an external help-seeking source, in relation to other internal help-seeking sources and affect components.

Advertisement

5. Conclusion

These findings contribute to the growing body of SRL literature because it demonstrates new ways students are regulating their actions and seeking for help in ITSs in relation to affect. The results from the present article showed that a pattern emerged in which students showed more executive help-seeking behaviors rather than instrumental ones. Students feeling bored, confused, and frustrated tended to use more hints, and they were less likely to switch to external help-seeking sources. Also, those feeling happy or engaged were less likely to use hints. It may be because students were feeling disengaged from the task or because of the level of difficulty of the task in the ITS. There is a need to research more units across time and investigate if these patterns hold.

Future research may assess students’ affective states which could shed light on the affective conditions that lead students to use hints and to profit from hint use. These results suggest a complex relationship between affect and hint use where affect explains variance in the tendency to use hints, but the directions of affect sometimes contradict the prior theory [52].

Moreover, future research may also focus on students’ help-seeking behaviors and affect in relation to their self-efficacy beliefs in ITSs. Efficacy beliefs influence peoples’ thinking process, goals, effort, commitment, outcomes, resilience, and how much stress or depression they tend to feel when performing a task [53]. Students’ usage of hints may be related to their sense of efficacy, and it may be a product of hint usage across the span of different units.

Advertisement

Acknowledgments

The authors gratefully acknowledge the support of the National Science Foundation (NSF) that funded this research. The collaboration of Steve Ritter and Tristan Nixon at Carnegie Learning is also especially appreciated regarding their assistance in adapting the Cognitive Tutor to include the instruments used in this study.

Advertisement

Declaration of interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. 1. Aleven V. Help seeking and intelligent tutoring systems: Theoretical perspectives and a step towards theoretical integration. In: Aleven V, Azevedo R, editors. International Handbook of Metacognition and Learning. New York: Springer; 2013. pp. 311-335
  2. 2. Bernacki ML, Nokes-Malach TJ, Aleven V. Fine-grained assessment of motivation over long periods of learning with an intelligent tutoring system: Methodology. advantages, and preliminary results. In: Aleven V, Azevedo R, editors. International Handbook of Metacognition and Learning. New York: Springer; 2013. pp. 629-644
  3. 3. Karabenick SA, Gonida EN. Academic help seeking as a self-regulated learning strategy. In: Schunk DH, Greene JA, editors. Handbook of Self-Regulation of Learning and Performance. New York: Routledge; 2018. pp. 421-433
  4. 4. Roll I, Aleven V, McLaren BM, Koedinger KR. Improving students’ help-seeking skills using metacognitive feedback in an intelligent tutoring system. Learning and Instruction. 2011;21(2):267-280. DOI: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2010.07.004
  5. 5. Otieno C, Schwonke R, Salden R, Renkl A. Can help seeking behavior in intelligent tutoring systems be used as online measure for goal orientation? Cognitive Science. 2013;35:1103-1108
  6. 6. Gonida EN, Karabenick SA, Makara KA, Hatzikyriakou GA. Perceived parent goals and student goal orientations as predictors of seeking or not seeking help: Does age matter? Learning and Instruction. 2014;33:120-130. DOI: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2014.04.007
  7. 7. Roll I, Baker RSJD, Aleven V, Koedinger KR. On the benefits of seeking (and avoiding) help in online problem-solving environments. Journal of the Learning Sciences. 2014;23(4):537-560. DOI: 10.1080/10508406.2014.883977
  8. 8. Schenke K, Lam AC, Conley AMM, Karabenick SA. Adolescents’ help seeking in mathematics classrooms: Relations between achievement and perceived classroom environmental influences over one school year. Contemporary Educational Psychology. 2015;41:133-146. DOI: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2015.01.003
  9. 9. Puustinen M, Bernicot J, Volckaert-Legrier O, Baker M. Naturally occurring help-seeking exchanges on a homework help forum. Computers and Education. 2015;81:89-101. DOI: 10.1016/j.compedu.2014.09.010
  10. 10. Shim SS, Rubenstein LDV, Drapeau CW. When perfectionism is coupled with low achievement: The effects on academic engagement and help seeking in middle school. Learning and Individual Differences. 2016;45:237-244. DOI: 10.1016/j.lindif.2015.12.016
  11. 11. Zander L, Chen IC, Hannover B. Who asks whom for help in mathematics? A sociometric analysis of adolescents’ help-seeking within and beyond clique boundaries. Learning and Individual Differences. 2019;72:49-58. DOI: 10.1016/j.lindif.2019.03.002
  12. 12. Du J, Xu J, Fan X. Investigating factors that influence students’ help seeking in math homework: A multilevel analysis. Learning and Individual Differences. 2016;48:29-35. DOI: 10.1016/j.lindif.2016.03.002
  13. 13. Roussel P, Elliot AJ, Feltman R. The influence of achievement goals and social goals on help-seeking from peers in an academic context. Learning and Instruction. 2011;21(3):394-402. DOI: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2010.05.003
  14. 14. Ryan AM, Shim SS. Changes in help seeking from peers during early adolescence: Associations with changes in achievement and perceptions of teachers. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2012;104(4):1122-1134. DOI: 10.1037/a0027696
  15. 15. Kiefer SM, Shim SS. Academic help seeking from peers during adolescence: The role of social goals. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology. 2016;42:80-88. DOI: 10.1016/j.appdev.2015.12.002
  16. 16. Shin H. The role of friends in help-seeking tendencies during early adolescence: Do classroom goal structures moderate selection and influence of friends? Contemporary Educational Psychology. 2018;53:135-145. DOI: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2018.03.002
  17. 17. Fletcher AK. Help seeking: Agentic learners initiating feedback. Educational Review. 2018;70(4):389-408. DOI: 10.1080/00131911.2017.1340871
  18. 18. Parker JS, Shum KZ, Suldo SM, Shaunessy-Dedrick EM, Ferron J, Dedrick RF. Predictors of adaptive help seeking across ninth-grade students enrolled in advanced placement and international baccalaureate courses. Psychology in the Schools. 2019;56(5):652-669. DOI: 10.1002/pits.22223
  19. 19. Ryan AM, Shin H. Help-seeking tendencies during early adolescence: An examination of motivational correlates and consequences for achievement. Learning and Instruction. 2011;21(2):247-256. DOI: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2010.07.003
  20. 20. Kessels U, Steinmayr R. Macho-man in school: Toward the role of gender role self-concepts and help seeking in school performance. Learning and Individual Differences. 2013;23(1):234-240. DOI: 10.1016/j.lindif.2012.09.013
  21. 21. Best P, Gil-Rodriguez E, Manktelow R, Taylor BJ. Seeking help from everyone and no-one: Conceptualizing the online help-seeking process among adolescent males. Qualitative Health Research. 2016;26(8):1067-1077. DOI: 10.1177/1049732316648128
  22. 22. Efklides A, Schwartz BL, Brown V. Motivation and affect in self-regulated learning: Does metacognition play a role? In: Schunk DH, Greene JA, editors. Handbook of Self-Regulation of Learning and Performance. New York: Routledge; 2018. pp. 64-82
  23. 23. D’Mello S. A selective metaanalysis on the relative incidence of discrete affective states during learning with technology. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2013;105(4):1082-1099
  24. 24. Arroyo I, Woolf BP, Burelson W, Muldner K, Rai D, Tai M. A multimedia adaptive tutoring system for mathematics that addresses cognition, metacognition and affect. International Journal of Artificial Intelligence in Education. 2014;24(4):387-426. DOI: 10.1007/s40593-014-0023-y
  25. 25. San Pedro MOZ, Baker RSJD, Rodrigo MMT. Carelessness and affect in an intelligent tutoring system for mathematics. International Journal of Artificial Intelligence in Education. 2014;24(2):189-210. DOI: 10.1007/s40593-014-0015-y
  26. 26. Tornare E, Czajkowski NO, Pons F. Children’s emotions in math problem solving situations: Contributions of self-concept, metacognitive experiences, and performance. Learning and Instruction. 2015;39:88-96. DOI: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2015.05.011
  27. 27. Ganotice FA, Datu JAD, King RB. Which emotional profiles exhibit the best learning outcomes? A person-centered analysis of students’ academic emotions. School Psychology International. 2016;37(5):498-518. DOI: 10.1177/0143034316660147
  28. 28. Padrón-Rivera G, Rebolledo-Mendez G, Parra PP, Huerta-Pacheco NS. Identification of action units related to affective states in a tutoring system for mathematics. Educational Technology and Society. 2016;19(2):77-86
  29. 29. Villavicencio FT, Bernardo ABI. Beyond math anxiety: Positive emotions predict mathematics achievement, self-regulation, and self-efficacy. Asia-Pacific Education Researcher. 2016;25(3):415-422. DOI: 10.1007/s40299-015-0251-4
  30. 30. Sainio PJ, Eklund KM, Ahonen TPS, Kiuru NH. The role of learning difficulties in adolescents’ academic emotions and academic achievement. Journal of Learning Disabilities. 2019, 2019;52(4):287-298. DOI: 10.1177/0022219419841567
  31. 31. Valinasab S, Zeinali A. The mediational pathway among academic emotions, self-regulated learning, and academic motivation with academic achievement of high school students. International Journal of Educational and Psychological Researches. 2018;3(4):255-261. DOI: 10.4103/jepr.jepr_3_17
  32. 32. Westphal A, Kretschmann J, Gronostaj A, Vock M. More enjoyment, less anxiety and boredom: How achievement emotions relate to academic self-concept and teachers’ diagnostic skills. Learning and Individual Differences. 2018;62:108-117. DOI: 10.1016/j.lindif.2018.01.016
  33. 33. Yu FY, Wu WS, Huang HC. Promoting middle school students’ learning motivation and academic emotions via student-created feedback for online student-created multiple-choice questions. Asia-Pacific Education Researcher. 2018;27(5):395-408. DOI: 10.1007/s40299-018-0398-x
  34. 34. Putwain DW, Becker S, Symes W, Pekrun R. Reciprocal relations between students’ academic enjoyment, boredom, and achievement over time. Learning and Instruction. 2018;54:73-81. DOI: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2017.08.004
  35. 35. Zakharov K, Mitrovic A, Johnston L. Towards emotionally-intelligent pedagogical agents. In: Woolf BP, Aïmeur E, Nkambou R, Lajoie S, editors. Proceedings of Intelligent Tutoring Systems, 9th International Conference, ITS 2008. Berlin, Germany: Springer; 2008. pp. 19-28. DOI: 10.1007/978-3-540-69132-7_7
  36. 36. Chaffar S, Derbali L, Frasson C. Inducing positive emotional state in intelligent tutoring systems. In: Dimitrova V, Mizoguchi R, Du Boulay B, Graesser A, editors. Proceedings of the 2009 Conference on Artificial Intelligence in Education: Building Learning System that Care: From Knowledge Representation to Affective Modelling; Amsterdam. Netherlands: IOS Press; 2009. pp. 716-718. Available from: https://ebooks.iospress.nl/publication/5255
  37. 37. Mao X, Li Z. Implementing emotion-based user-aware e-learning. In: Proceedings of the 27th International Conference Extended Abstracts on Human Factors in Computing Systems. New York, NY: ACM Press; Apr 2009. pp. 3787-3792. Available from: https://dl.acm.org/doi/proceedings/10.1145/3334480
  38. 38. Woolf B, Burleson W, Arroyo I, Dragon T, Cooper D, Picard R. Affect-aware tutors: Recognising and responding to student affect. International Journal of Learning Technology. 2009;4(3/4):129-164. DOI: 10.1504/IJLT.2009.028804
  39. 39. Strain A, D‘Mello S. Emotion regulation during learning. In: Biswas G, Bull S, Kay J, Mitrovic A, editors. Proceedings of Artificial Intelligence in Education, 15th International Conference, AIED 2011. Berlin, Germany: Springer; 2011. pp. 566-568. DOI: 10.1007/978-3-642-21869-9_103
  40. 40. Rodrigo MMT, Baker RSJD, Agapito J, Nabos J, Repalam MC, Reyes SS, et al. The effects of an interactive software agent on student affective dynamics while using. An intelligent tutoring system. IEEE Transactions on Affective Computing. 2012;3(2):224-236. Available from https://ieeexplore.ieee.org/document/6122013
  41. 41. D’Mello S, Graesser A. AutoTutor and affective AutoTutor: Learning by talking with cognitively and emotionally intelligent computers that talk back. ACM Transactions on Interactive Intelligent Systems. 2012;2(4):1-39. DOI: 10.1145/2395123.2395128
  42. 42. Tian F, Gao P, Li L, Zhang W, Liang H, Qian Y, et al. Recognizing and regulating e-learners’ emotions based on interactive Chinese texts in e-learning systems. Knowledge-Based Systems. 2014;55:148-164. DOI: 10.1016/j.knosys.2013.10.019
  43. 43. Malekzadeh M, Mustafa MB, Lahsasna A. A review of emotion regulation in intelligent tutoring systems. Educational Technology and Society. 2015;18(4):435-445. Available from: http://www.jstor.org/stable/jeductechsoci.18.4.435
  44. 44. Gordon G, Spaulding S, Korywestlund J, Lee JJ, Plummer L, Martinez M, et al. Affective personalization of a social robot tutor for children’s second language skills. In: Proceedings of the 30th AAAI Conference on Artificial Intelligence. Palo Alto, CA: MIT; 2016. pp. 3951-3957. DOI: 10.1609/aaai.v30i1.9914
  45. 45. Aleven V, Roll I, McLaren BM, Koedinger KR. Help helps, but only so much: Research on help seeking with intelligent tutoring systems. International Journal of Artificial Intelligence in Education Society. 2016;26(1):205-223. DOI: 10.1007/s40593-015-0089-1
  46. 46. Ritter S, Anderson JR, Koedinger KR, Corbett A. The cognitive tutor: Applied research in mathematics education. Psychonomics Bulletin & Review. 2007;14(2):249-255. DOI: 10.3758/BF03194060
  47. 47. Bernacki ML, Nokes-Malach TJ, Aleven V. Examining self-efficacy during learning: Variability and relations to behavior, performance, and learning. Metacognition and Learning. 2015;10(1):99-117. DOI: 10.1007/s11409-014-9127-x
  48. 48. Bernacki M, Ritter S. Hopewell 2011-2012 Algebra (Learning Orientation Survey). Dataset 718 in DataShop. 2013. Available from: https://pslcdatashop.web.cmu.edu/DatasetInfo?datasetId=718
  49. 49. Aleven V, McLaren BM, Roll I, Koedinger KR. Toward meta-cognitive tutoring: A model of help-seeking with a cognitive tutor. International Journal of Artificial Intelligence. 2006;16(2):101-130
  50. 50. Rubin DB. Inference and missing data. Biometrika. 1976;63(3):581-592. DOI: 10.2307/2335739
  51. 51. Little RJA. A test of missing completely at random for multivariate data with missing values. Journal of the American Statistical Association. 1988;83(404):1198-1202. DOI: 10.1080/01621459.1988.10478722
  52. 52. Karabenick SA. Classroom and technology-supported help seeking: The need for converging research paradigms. Learning and Instruction. 2011;21(2):290-296. DOI: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2010.07.007
  53. 53. Bandura A. Exercise of human agency through collective efficacy. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2000;9(3):75-78. Available from: http://www.jstor.org/stable/20182630

Written By

Ana Paula S. Loures-Elias and Matthew L. Bernacki

Submitted: 13 November 2023 Reviewed: 28 December 2023 Published: 09 February 2024